Dehaene–Changeux model (DCM)
The Dehaene–Changeux model (DCM), also known as the global neuronal workspace or the global cognitive workspace model is a part of Bernard Baars's "global workspace model" for consciousness.
It is a computer model of the neural correlates of consciousness programmed as a neural network. It attempts to reproduce the swarm behaviour[clarification needed] of the brain's higher cognitive functions such as consciousness, decision-making[1] and the central executive functions.

The Dehaene–Changeux model (DCM), also known as the global neuronal workspace or the global cognitive workspace model is a part of Bernard Baars's "global workspace model" for consciousness.

It is a computer model of the neural correlates of consciousness programmed as a neural network. It attempts to reproduce the swarm behaviour[clarification needed] of the brain's higher cognitive functions such as consciousnessdecision-making[1] and the central executive functions. It was developed by cognitive neuroscientists Stanislas Dehaene and Jean-Pierre Changeux beginning in 1986.[2] It has been used to provide a predictive framework to the study of inattentional blindness and the solving of the Tower of London test.[3][4]

 

Contents

  [hide
  • 1 History
  • 2 Structure
    • 2.1 General structure
    • 2.2 A swarm and a multi-agent system composed of neural networks
  • 3 Behavior
  • 4 Scholarly reception
    • 4.1 Self-organized criticality
    • 4.2 States of consciousness and phenomenology
  • 5 Publications
  • 6 See also
  • 7 References
  • 8 External links

 

History[edit]

The Dehaene–Changeux model was initially established as a spin glass neural network attempting to represent learning and to then provide a stepping stone towards artificial learning among other objectives. It would later be used to predict observable reaction times within the priming paradigm[5] and in inattentional blindness.

Structure[edit]

General structure[edit]

The Dehaene–Changeux model is a meta neural network (i.e. a network of neural networks) composed of a very large number of integrate-and-fire neurons programmed in either a stochastic or deterministic way. The neurons are organised in complex thalamo-cortical columns with long-range connexions and a critical role[clarification needed] played by the interaction between von Economo's areas. Each thalamo-cortical column is composed of pyramidal cells and inhibitory interneurons receiving a long-distance excitatory neuromodulation which could represent noradrenergic input.

A swarm and a multi-agent system composed of neural networks[edit]

Among others Cohen & Hudson (2002) had already used "Meta neural networks as intelligent agents for diagnosis "[6] Similarly to Cohen & Hudson, Dehaene & Changeux have established their model as an interaction of meta-neural networks (thalamocortical columns) themselves programmed in the manner of a "hierarchy of neural networks that together act as an intelligent agent", in order to use them as a system composed of a large scale of inter-connected intelligent agents for predicting the self-organized behaviour of the neural correlates of consciousness. It may also be noted that Jain et al. (2002) had already clearly identified spiking neurons as intelligent agents[7] since the lower bound for computational power of networks of spiking neurons is the capacity to simulate in real-time for boolean-valued inputs any Turing machine.[8] The DCM being composed of a very large number of interacting sub-networks which are themselves intelligent agents, it is formally a Multi-agent system programmed as a Swarm or neural networks and a fortiori of spiking neurons.

The three levels of complexity of the global workspace model: the integrate-and-fire neuron, the thalamo-cortical bundle and the long-range connexion. The authors provide the legend: "Shown are the constituents of the simulation (upper diagrams) and typical patterns of spontaneous activity that they can produce (lower tracings). We simulated a nested architecture in which spiking neurons (A) are incorporated within thalamocortical columns (B), which are themselves interconnected hierarchically by local and long-distance cortical connections (C) (see Materials and methods for details). While single neurons may generate sustained oscillations of membrane potentials (A), only the column and network levels generate complex waxing-and-waning EEG-like oscillations (B) and metastable global states of sustained firing (C)."[9]

Behavior[edit]

The DCM exhibits several surcritical[clarification needed] emergent behaviors such as multistability and a Hopf bifurcation between two very different regimes which may represent either sleep or arousal with a various all-or-none behaviors which Dehaene et al. use to determine a testable taxonomy between different states of consciousness.[10][clarification needed]

Scholarly reception[edit]

Self-organized criticality[edit]

The Dehaene-Changeux Model contributed to the study of nonlinearity and self-organized criticality in particular as an explanatory model of the brain's emergent behaviors, including consciousness. Studying the brain's phase-locking and large-scale synchronization, Kitzbichler et al. (2011a) confirmed that criticality is a property of human brain functional network organization at all frequency intervals in the brain's physiological bandwidth.[11]

Furthermore, exploring the neural dynamics of cognitive efforts after, inter alia, the Dehaene-Changeux Model, Kitzbichler et al. (2011b) demonstrated how cognitive effort breaks the modularity of mind to make human brain functional networks transiently adopt a more efficient but less economical configuration.[12] Werner (2007a) used the Dehaene-Changeux Global Neuronal Workspace to defend the use of statistical physics approaches for exploring phase transitions, scaling and universality properties of the do-called "Dynamic Core" of the brain, with relevance to the macroscopic electrical activity in EEG and EMG.[13] Furthermore building from the Dehaene-Changeux Model, Werner (2007b) proposed that the application of the twin concepts of scaling and universality of the theory of non-equilibrium phase transitions can serve as an informative approach for elucidating the nature of underlying neural-mechanisms, with emphasis on the dynamics of recursively reentrant activity flow in intracortical and cortico-subcortical neuronal loops. Friston (2000) also claimed that "the nonlinear nature of asynchronous coupling enables the rich, context-sensitive interactions that characterize real brain dynamics, suggesting that it plays a role in functional integration that may be as important as synchronous interactions".[14]

States of consciousness and phenomenology[edit]

It contributed to the study of phase transition in the brain under sedation, and notably GABA-ergic sedation such as that induced by propofol (Murphy et al. 2011, Stamatakis et al. 2010).[15][16] The Dehaene-Changeux Model was contrasted and cited in the study of collective consciousness and its pathologies (Wallace et al. 2007).[17] Boly et al. (2007) used the model for a reverse somatotopic study, demonstrating a correlation between baseline brain activity and somatosensory perception in humans.[18] Boly et al. (2008) also used the DCM in a study of the baseline state of consciousness of the human brain's default network.[19]

Publications[edit]

  • Naccache, L. Cognitive aging considered from the point of view of cognitive neurosciences of consciousness. Psychologie & NeuroPsychiatrie du vieillissement. Volume 5, Number 1, 17–21, Mars 2007
  • Rialle, V and Stip, E. Cognitive modeling in psychiatry: from symbolic models to parallel and distributed models J Psychiatry Neurosci. 1994 May; 19(3): 178–192.
  • Ravi Prakash, Om Prakash, Shashi Prakash, Priyadarshi Abhishek, and Sachin Gandotra Global workspace model of consciousness and its electromagnetic correlates Ann Indian Acad Neurol. 2008 Jul–Sep; 11(3): 146–153. doi:10.4103/0972-2327.42933
  • Zigmond, Michael J.(1999) Fundamental neuroscience', Academic Press p1551
  • Bernard J. Baars,Nicole M. Gage Cognition, brain, and consciousness: introduction to cognitive neuroscience Academic Press, 2010 p. 287
  • Carlos Hernández, Ricardo Sanz, Jaime Gómez-Ramirez, Leslie S. Smith, Amir Hussain, Antonio Chella, Igor AleksanderFrom Brains to Systems: Brain-Inspired Cognitive Systems' Volume 718 of Advances in Experimental Medicine and Biology SeriesSpringer, 2011 p. 230
  • Steven Laureys et al. The boundaries of consciousness: neurobiology and neuropathology Volume 150 of Progress in brain research Elsevier, 2006 p. 45
  • Michael S. Gazzaniga The cognitive neurosciences MIT Press, 2004 p.1146
  • Stanislas Dehaene The cognitive neuroscience of consciousness MIT Press 2001 p.13
  • Tim Bayne, Axel Cleeremans, Patrick Wilken The Oxford companion to consciousness Oxford University Press 2009 p.332
  • Hans Liljenström, Peter Århem Consciousness transitions: phylogenetic, ontogenetic, and physiological aspects Elsevier 2008 p. 126

See also[edit]

 

References[edit]

  1. Jump up^ Dehaene S, Changeux JP. Reward-dependent learning in neuronal networks for planning and decision making.Prog Brain Res. 2000;126:217-29.
  2. Jump up^ Dehaene S, Changeux JP. Experimental and theoretical approaches to conscious processing. Neuron. 2011 Apr 28;70(2):200-27.
  3. Jump up^ Changeux JP, Dehaene S. Hierarchical neuronal modeling of cognitive functions: from synaptic transmission to the Tower of London. C R Acad Sci III. 1998 Feb–Mar;321(2–3):241-7.
  4. Jump up^ Dehaene S, Changeux JP, Nadal JP. Neural networks that learn temporal sequences by selection. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1987 May;84(9):2727-31.
  5. Jump up^ Epub 2010 Jan 25. Van den Bussche E, Hughes G, Humbeeck NV, Reynvoet B The relation between consciousness and attention: an empirical study using the priming paradigm. Conscious Cogn. 2010 Mar;19(1):86–97..
  6. Jump up^ Cohen, M.E.; Hudson, D.L.; Meta neural networks as intelligent agents for diagnosis Neural Networks, 2002. IJCNN '02. Proceedings of the 2002 International Joint Conference on 233 – 238
  7. Jump up^ L. C. Jain, Zhengxin Chen, Nikhil Ichalkaranje Intelligent agents and their applications Volume 98 de Studies in fuzziness and soft computing
  8. Jump up^ Maas, W. Lower Bound for the computational power of networks of spking neurons [1]
  9. Jump up^ Dehaene S, Changeux J-P (2005) Ongoing Spontaneous Activity Controls Access to Consciousness: A Neuronal Model for Inattentional Blindness. PLoS Biol 3(5): e141.doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0030141 image in full open source
  10. Jump up^ Dehaene S, Changeux JP, Naccache L, Sackur J, Sergent C. Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: a testable taxonomy. Trends Cogn Sci. 2006 May;10(5):204-11. Epub 2006 Apr 17.
  11. Jump up^ Kitzbichler MG, Smith ML, Christensen SR, Bullmore E.Broadband criticality of human brain network synchronization. PLoS Comput Biol. 2009 Mar;5(3):e1000314. Epub 2009 Mar 20.
  12. Jump up^ Kitzbichler MG, Henson RN, Smith ML, Nathan PJ, Bullmore ET. Cognitive effort drives workspace configuration of human brain functional networks. J Neurosci. 2011 Jun 1;31(22):8259-70.
  13. Jump up^ Werner G. Brain dynamics across levels of organization.J Physiol Paris. 2007 Jul–Nov;101(4–6):273-9. Epub 2008 Jan 8.
  14. Jump up^ Friston KJ. The labile brain. I . Neuronal transients and nonlinear coupling. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2000 Feb 29;355(1394):215-36.
  15. Jump up^ Murphy M, Bruno MA, Riedner BA, Boveroux P, Noirhomme Q, Landsness EC, Brichant JF, Phillips C, Massimini M, Laureys S, Tononi G, Boly M.Propofol anesthesia and sleep: a high-density EEG study. Sleep. 2011 Mar 1;34(3):283-91A.
  16. Jump up^ Stamatakis EA, Adapa RM, Absalom AR, Menon DK.Changes in resting neural connectivity during propofol sedation. PLoS One. 2010 Dec 2;5(12):e14224.
  17. Jump up^ Wallace RM, Fullilove MT, Fullilove RE, Wallace DN.Collective consciousness and its pathologies: understanding the failure of AIDS control and treatment in the United States. Theor Biol Med Model. 2007 Feb 26;4:10.
  18. Jump up^ Boly M, Balteau E, Schnakers C, Degueldre C, Moonen G, Luxen A, Phillips C, Peigneux P, Maquet P, Laureys S.Baseline brain activity fluctuations predict somatosensory perception in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007 Jul 17;104(29):12187-92. Epub 2007 Jul 6.
  19. Jump up^ Boly M, Phillips C, Tshibanda L, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Schabus M, Dang-Vu TT, Moonen G, Hustinx R, Maquet P, Laureys S. Intrinsic brain activity in altered states of consciousness: how conscious is the default mode of brain function? Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1129:119-29. Review.

External links[edit]

PAGE NAVIGATOR(Help)
-
Machine Learning Methods & Algorithms »Machine Learning Methods & Algorithms
Supervised learning »Supervised learning
Artificial neural network »Artificial neural network
Dehaene–Changeux model (DCM)
+Commentaar (0)
+Citaten (0)
+About